(Image: Design Pics Inc/Rex Features)
The set of genes that help create our most grievous frailties may
also underlie our greatest strengths – and sometimes the choice is
settled in childhood
WHY
are some children better at sharing than others? One attempt to find
out uses what you could call the "Bamba test". In a large,
playroom-like lab, a 3-year-old spends an hour or so playing games with
a friendly woman, before snack time is announced. The adult brings out
two packs of Bambas - peanut-butter-flavoured corn puffs much coveted
in this part of the world.
The
child's pack, like every normal one, holds 24 of the treats. But when
the woman opens hers, she dumps out the contents and cries: "Mine has
only three!" Will the 3-year-old share without being asked?
Most
do not. "Self-initiated sharing is difficult," says psychologist Arial
Knafo, who runs this study at The Hebrew University of Jerusalem in
Israel. "You have to detect the need, then decide to do it."
A
few 3-year-olds, however, will offer up their Bambas. What's different
about them? The children most likely to share carried a certain gene
variant, the "7R" version of DRD4, a gene that affects levels of the important brain chemical dopamine.
What made this finding remarkable was that this gene variant has generally been tied to antisocial
behaviour. A pile of previous studies found that children with the 7R
variant were more likely to be naughty and hyperactive. It had been
dubbed the ADHD gene, the brat gene, the drinking gene, even the slut
gene. Now Knafo was effectively calling it the Bamba-sharing gene. The
bad-news gene was having a good effect.
This
apparently paradoxical result lies at the heart of a major revision
taking place in behavioural science - a recasting of the "vulnerability
gene" model of many mood and behavioural disorders. This model,
tremendously influential in psychiatry and psychology, has arisen over
the past couple of decades as research tied several gene variants to
high risk of mood and behavioural troubles, such as depression,
aggression or, in the case of DRD4, attention and conduct disorders.
Crucially,
these genes only caused problems when combined with a difficult
childhood. Often termed vulnerability (or risk) genes, they have been
held up as a prime model of how genes interact with environment to
affect mood and behaviour.
But
might we have got these genes all wrong? A fresh look at the evidence
is suggesting that in fact they often create greater strength and
happiness in people who have fortunate childhoods. The so-called
vulnerability genes, in short, make you more attuned and responsive to
your environment, whether bad or good.
"These
genes aren't about risk," says Jay Belsky, a psychologist at the
University of California, Davis, who helped establish what is being
called the plasticity gene hypothesis, among other terms. "It is
responsiveness - for better or worse."
The
genetics of behaviour has always been a controversial field, with
far-reaching implications for individuals and society. The origins of
the vulnerability gene model lie in a study published in 1996 that
showed we have a greater risk of becoming depressed or anxious if we
have a certain version of a gene called SERT. The short version
of this gene, which is carried by 30 to 50 per cent of people, lowers
levels of serotonin, another brain chemical.
Serotonin
was already much on the minds of psychiatrists and drug companies. The
introduction of Prozac had created a growing class of antidepressants,
called selective serotonin re-uptake inhibitors (SSRIs), thought to
work by raising serotonin levels. The short version of SERT was quickly dubbed "the depression gene". Yet that label had to be too simplistic. A short SERT does not guarantee depression, it only raises the risk. Environmental factors also had to play a role.
That
crucial mingling of nature and nurture seemed to be neatly elucidated a
few years later by two seminal studies from epidemiologists Avshalom
Caspi and Terrie Moffitt of King's College, London. They found that a
short SERT raised people's risk of depression only if they suffered rough childhoods or episodes of intense stress as adults (Science, vol 301, p 386). They also showed that a variant of a gene called MAOA,
which affects serotonin and several other brain chemicals, increased
the chance of violent or sociopathic behaviour, but only in people who
were abused as children (Science, vol 297, p 851).
The
two papers attracted enormous attention and spurred many further
studies. While not all replicated Caspi and Moffitt's results, many
did, and so the vulnerability gene model came to dominate the field of
behavioural genetics.
But
not everyone was buying it. Back in 1995, W. Thomas Boyce, a child
development specialist then at the University of California, Berkeley,
had been trying to understand why some children seemed to react more to
their environment in measures ranging from heartbeat and blood pressure
to levels of cortisol, a hormone related to stress. Boyce examined how
this reactivity affected rates of asthma in children aged 3 to 5. While
some had the same rates of illness regardless of their home life, more
reactive kids had worse asthma if they lived in stressful environments
and less asthma if in low-stress homes. They were simply more sensitive
to their environment, whether bad or good.
Upside ignored
Boyce
was soon joined in this line of inquiry by Bruce Ellis at the
University of Arizona in Tucson. Together they speculated that this
reactivity also affects mood and behaviour. Drawing on Swedish terms,
they distinguished between "dandelion children", who did about the same
whatever their environment, and "orchid children", who wilted under
poor care but flourished if carefully tended (Development and Psychopathology, vol 17, p 271).
Then,
in 1997, Belsky also raised the idea of children who were especially
sensitive to their early environments. Initially unaware of Boyce and
Ellis's work, he was trying to figure out why some troubled kids
responded more than others to counselling or other interventions to
change their behaviour.
As
Belsky, Boyce and Ellis watched the vulnerability-gene studies
accumulate, they realised these could be the very genes that prompted
the sensitivity they had found. And when Belsky delved into the
literature he found evidence showing exactly that. Many
vulnerability-gene studies indeed seemed to show that the so-called bad
variants of SERT, DRD4, and MAOA generated extra
resilience and other assets in people with fortunate early years. Yet
the literature largely ignored this upside: in paper after paper, the
raw data and graphs indicated the positive effects, but the text failed
to explore or even note them.
Belsky
was quite happy to note them. In 2006, he and others began publishing
new studies and re-analyses of old ones showing that the so-called
vulnerability genes created not just risk but bidirectional sensitivity.
Other groups started to investigate the idea. Knafo, for instance, began exploring DRD4's
effect on social behaviour, as described earlier. He has shown that as
long as parenting is good, toddlers with the 7R variant are more
"prosocial" than those with the more common 4R form (Development and Psychopathology,
vol 23, p 53). The orchids not only shared their Bambas, but were also
more likely to pick up a researcher's dropped pencil, express sympathy
over a bumped knee, and help find and then comfort a lost doll.
Knafo
found that this edge in sociability and generosity increased over the
three years he followed the children. This may simply reflect the
natural course of child development. Or it may reflect a positive
feedback loop, as both responsive child and engaged parent react to
their good chemistry. Some of the mothers probably carry the 7R variant
too, since a 7R child must have at least one 7R parent. Knafo hadn't
genotyped them, so he can't say.
In 2008, another team showed that DRD4's
7R variant does not just make its bearers more responsive to natural
variations in their upbringing; those carrying it also respond more to
experimental interventions. In a programme for mothers of difficult
toddlers that trained them to be more engaged and attentive, children
carrying the 7R variant benefited the most (Developmental Psychology, vol 44, p 293).
Belsky,
meanwhile, is doing bigger studies that gauge the cumulative effects of
several plasticity genes. In 2010, he published an analysis drawn from
a 12-year study of 1586 adolescents. He analysed five genes (SERT, MAOA, DRD4,
and two other genes that regulate dopamine) and collected data on the
teens' behaviour and self-control, and on the mothers' engagement in
their lives.
The
numbers, once crunched, showed no significant effects on girls. But the
754 boys did react differently according to their genes, showing
distinct dandelion or orchid effects (Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
vol 52, p 619). The boys with no or only one plasticity variant proved
to be dandelions: they fared about the same regardless of how engaged
their mothers were. Those with two to five plasticity variants,
however, responded like orchids, and the more they had, the more
sensitive they were (see diagram).
With
the caveat that this kind of multiple-gene study has not yet been
replicated, the effect found seems to fit the orchid hypothesis
beautifully. It also accords with the principle that complex traits are
generally determined by many genes, not just one.
In
addition, the lack of effect in boys with just one plasticity variant
offers an explanation for a puzzle hanging over the field for years:
the failure of some single-gene studies to show the expected effects.
Belsky's research suggests that the effect of any one gene depends on
the others.
Support
is coming from other fields as well. Orchid-dandelion effects have been
found in rhesus monkeys, the only primate besides humans to have the
short SERT variant. Ethologist Stephen Suomi of the US National
Institutes of Health in Bethesda, Maryland, has done experiments with
these monkeys that cannot be done with people, such as swapping babies
at birth to switch the kind of parenting they receive. Unlike dandelion
monkeys, orchid monkeys tend to turn out neurotic if raised by
insecure, neurotic mothers but resilient and self-assured if raised by
secure, competent mothers.
The
orchid hypothesis also meshes with observations of adults in
psychotherapy. Since 1997, Californian psychiatrists Elaine and Arthur
Aron have written about what they call "highly sensitive persons", or
HSPs, who are especially responsive not just to trouble but to many of
life's pleasures and subtleties. As Elaine Aron sees it, this group,
comprising an estimated 15 to 20 per cent of the population, perceive
life at a finer, more nuanced scale.
As
the plasticity theory gained ground, the Arons and others have wondered
if HSPs are essentially orchid children grown up. They argue that HSPs
share with the orchid children a particularly reactive physiological
and sensory response to the world.
Now
the first genetic evidence is emerging to support that view. One set of
preliminary results, presented at last year's annual meeting of the US
Society of Biological Psychiatry in San Francisco, found that HSPs were
more likely to carry the short SERT gene (see bit.ly/thFP3i). Another, also published in 2011, correlated HSP characteristics with 10 genetic variations that affect dopamine levels (PloS One, vol 6, p e21636).
Evolutionary asset
All
this work from different fields has given the plasticity hypothesis far
more traction in the past two to three years. "This thing is just
exploding," as Belsky puts it.
The
idea is taking hold fastest among specialists in child development. At
the 2009 biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child
Development, the plasticity hypothesis was mentioned only in passing.
At the subsequent meeting, in 2011, it was the main subject of half a
dozen sessions. Meanwhile, the February 2011 issue of Development and Psychopathology carried a special section with a dozen papers on it (vol 23, p 1).
The
plasticity hypothesis is also gaining ground in fields such as adult
psychology, behavioural genetics and anthropology. Five years ago you
could count on your fingers the researchers doing this work, says
Belsky. Now dozens have written papers, which "come out so fast now,
that when I write review papers, the review is outdated before it's
printed".
Another
telling sign came in 2010, when the originator of the
vulnerability-gene hypothesis was converted. Klaus-Peter Lesch, a
psychiatrist at the University of Würzburg in Germany, who discovered
the short SERT gene in 1995, wrote a paper titled "Looking on the bright side of transporter gene variation". In it, he called for the short SERT to be seen as an evolutionary asset.
In
fact, many of the orchid-theory supporters argue that even with its
drawbacks, sensitivity is more often than not adaptive - and therefore
selected for. This idea has gained credence by the discovery over the
last decade that many of the plasticity genes have spread rapidly
through humankind over the last 50,000 years.
Of the leading orchid-gene variants - the short SERT, the 7R DRD4 and the more plastic version of MAOA
- none existed in humans 80,000 years ago. Since emerging, these
variants have spread into 20 to 50 per cent of the population. "That's
not random drift," says John Hawks, an evolutionary anthropologist at
the University of Wisconsin-Madison. "They're being selected for."
Orchid
genes could provide an advantage in several ways. To start with, they
seem to create better mental health and greater resilience in people
with secure, stimulating childhoods. The "problem" traits they can
generate, such as anxiety, aggression or ADHD, could help survival in
conflict-ridden or volatile environments. Plasticity genes also boost
resilience at the group level by creating a mix of steady do-ers
(dandelions) and individuals with greater behavioural range (orchids).
Some
evolutionary anthropologists argue that these traits, particularly the
restlessness and risk-taking found in many carriers of the 7R DRD4,
may have helped drive human expansion. Today the 7R variant is most
common in populations that migrated fastest and furthest from Africa (American Journal of Physical Anthropology, vol 145, p 382).
It
may seem odd to link such achievements to a child's willingness to
share Bambas. Yet if the orchid hypothesis is right, the genes that
help create some of our most grievous frailties - anxiety and
aggression, melancholia and murder - may also underlie our greatest
strengths, from the sharing of meals to our spread around the globe.
Something to ponder next time you're offered a sweet.
David Dobbs is writing his fourth book, The Orchid and the Dandelion. You can find more of his writing at daviddobbs.net
http://www.newscientist.com/
No comments:
Post a Comment